Published: May 25, 2012

Nate and Zach Huey are identical, 15-year-old twins, who, like most twins, are somewhat dissimilar.

Nate runs cross country and track at Westminster High School. He specializes in mid-distance events like the two-mile run. Zach was a sprinter but suffered an injury that sidelined him.

Nate is learning the guitar but doesn’t read music. He plays by “tab” (drawings showing where to place fingers on the fretboard). Zach reads music and plays trombone in the band. He enjoys it but likes other instruments, too.

But the twins have much in common. Both like Japanese comic books called Manga. Both read voraciously and have a vocabulary that shows it. And both have been studied since the age of 4 by researchers at the at the 鶹Ѱ.

Much of the institute’s work focuses on identical and fraternal twins, who help scientists understand the role of nature and nurture. Reading development and reading disabilities are the focus of two twin studies conducted through the institute.

One of these has been funded since 1990 as the . It was recently awarded a $9.3 million grant from the U.S. National Institutes of Health for a five-year continuation of its research on the causes and treatment of learning disabilities in children and adolescents. As of last year, the center had tested 2,089 pairs of twins.

The Huey brothers are participants in a different study, the Longitudinal Twin Study of Early Reading Development, also under the auspices of the Institute for Behavioral Genetics. This study focuses on individual differences in reading and related skills in the general population from preschool age 4 years through age 15 years.

Nate and Zach clearly do not struggle with learning disabilities. While discussing the appeal of literature, Nate mentions “éԴdzܱ𳾱Գ,” a literary term describing the conclusion of a story. That word is, it’s safe to say, not commonly found in a high-schooler’s Twitter feed.

Nate says he “just sucks stuff like that up,” appreciating the power of a word that fits perfectly into a sentence. “A lot of kids at my school would call me a nerd, but I don’t really care.”

Similarly, Zach absorbs new words like a dry sponge in rain. In school, he enjoys vocabulary lessons. Asked to name some words he’s learned recently, he cites “hindrance,” “gluttony” and “avarice,” terms not usually heard rolling off the tongues of 15-year-olds.

Zach wants to express himself more precisely than do many of his peers. “I see people who actually strive to write unintelligibly,” he says.

While Nate and Zach both clearly excel in reading and writing, scientists do not know the degree to which their specific similarity in this area is due to genetic or environmental influences. However, a comparison of similarities for large samples of identical and fraternal twin pairs can yield estimates of the average influences of genes and environment in the population.

Identical twins have identical DNA, which is sometimes called the building block of life. DNA is a huge molecule that exists in all living cells. It carries a person’s genetic code, which influences a broad range of individual differences from eye color to a person’s capacity to decode language.

Fraternal twins share half their genes on average, the same as ordinary siblings. Since both the identical and fraternal twin pairs in the study share the same family environment, it is possible to infer the balance of influences from genes, shared family environment (such as books in the home) and non-shared environment (such as different peers or teachers).

Since 1990, researchers at the Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center have worked to understand why some children have more trouble than others learning to read. The group made impressive strides, but much - including the specific genetic sequences affecting reading ability - has yet to be determined.

Richard Olson, 鶹Ѱprofessor of , directs the center, which comprises six research projects on learning and reading. Investigators with complementary expertise examine learning disabilities from different perspectives across the six projects. The center houses researchers with specific knowledge of DNA analysis, behavior genetics, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and general aspects of reading.

In addition to center researchers at the CU-Boulder campus (Olson, Erik Willcutt, Sally Wadsworth and John DeFries), the center research team includes additional collaborators at the University of Denver (Janice Keenan and Bruce Pennington), the University of Nebraska (Shelley Smith) and Yale University (Jeffrey Gruen).

By sharing data gathered in the center’s laboratories, its researchers develop “broader perspectives on learning disabilities,” Olson said. “Kids have difficulty reading or excel in reading for many different reasons, and we want to understand that range.”

Environmental influences, Olson said, can be “just as important as knowing about genetic influences, and sometimes more important.”

To help improve the learning environment for children with reading disabilities, Investigators in the center have explored the use of computer programs to support basic reading skills and more accurate reading practice with speech support for difficult words. This tool was developed by Barbara Wise (formerly a researcher at the center) and her colleagues. Wise also consults with school districts and businesses that want to improve their reading programs.

And for Nate Huey, being tested for more than a decade was no bother. “I never really thought of it as a waste of time or anything. They’re doing research, which is valuable.”